An Open Letter to Our Students: Doing Interdisciplinary Moral Psychology
This chapter offers some recommendations and guiding principles for conducting interdisciplinary moral psychology research, which will benefit students and experienced scholars alike. It is especially helpful to scholars in the humanities looking to apply scientific methods to their work. Drawing from work at the intersection of philosophy and the cognitive and neural sciences, this chapter offers valuable advice for how to critically evaluate the scientific literature and avoid common pitfalls.
This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.
Access this chapter
Similar content being viewed by others
Moral Cognition: Introduction
Chapter © 2015
Moral Philosophy and the ‘Ethical Turn’ in Anthropology
Article Open access 01 August 2019
Moving beyond dichotomies: Liao, S. Matthew (ed.), Moral Brains: The Neuroscience of Morality, Oxford University Press, 2016
Article 16 September 2017
Notes
We dedicate this paragraph to our good friend Shaun Nichols.
What is the relation between p-values and power? If there is no effect to be detected (i.e., if the null hypothesis is true), then the p-value is uniformly distributed between 0 and 1 (i.e., one is equally likely to get a p-value between 0 and 0.2, between 0.2 and 0.4, etc.), independently of the power of the experiment. If the null hypothesis is false, the larger the power, the more likely it is that one will observe a small p-value (holding constant the effect size used to compute the power of the experiment).
More precisely, it is the probability of obtaining a statistic (a function of the data such as t or F) or a larger one if the null hypothesis is true.
In a recent article, Benjamin et al. (2017) have argued that the 0.05 significance level is insufficiently strict, and have recommended to decrease it to 0.005. They argue that findings at the 0.05 level provide too little evidence and that this lax significance level contributes to the current replication crisis in psychology and other sciences. One of us (EM) is a coauthor of this article and would like the 0.005 significance level to be widely accepted. For discussion, see however Amrhein and Greenland (2017); Lakens et al. (2017); McShane et al. (2017).
Since we’re trying to be encouraging and cheerful, we’ve relegated this advice (15) to a footnote: when you collaborate, be very explicit about work responsibilities and authorial order, from the outset. We’ve seen, and been party to, more than one unfortunate misunderstanding about contributions and credit, and they’re no fun at all. While the occasional tiff is likely unavoidable, hammering out expected contributions and credit before serious work begins is a very useful preventative. Potential discomfort isn’t a reason not to collaborate, but it is a reason to be very clear.
A draft of this paper was presented to the Autumn 2015 meeting of the Moral Psychology Research Group. Many thanks to participants for comments, especially Fiery Cushman, Valerie Tiberius, Maria Merritt, Eddy Nahmias, and Shaun Nichols. We also would like to thank Wesley Buckwalter, David Danks, Benjamin Voyer, and Wayne Wu for comments and suggestions.
References
- Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N. (2007). Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 569–576. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Amrhein, V., & Greenland, S. (2017). Remove, rather than redefine, statistical significance. Nature Human Behaviour. Google Scholar
- Bargh, J. A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–244. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Bedke, M. S. (2012). Against normative naturalism. Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 90, 111–129. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Bell, B. E., & Loftus, E. F. (1989). Trivial persuasion in the courtroom: The power of (a few) minor details. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 669. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Benjamin, D. J., Berger, J. O., Johannesson, M., Nosek, B. A., Wagenmakers, E.--J., Berk, R., Bollen, K. A., Brembs, B., Brown, L., Camerer, C., Cesarini, D., Chambers, C. D., Clyde, M., Cook, T. D., De Boeck, P., Dienes, Z., Dreber, A., Easwaran, K., Efferson, C., Fehr, E., Fidler, F., Field, A. P., Forster, M., George, E. I., Gonzalez, R., Goodman, S., Green, E., Green, D. P., Greenwald, A., Hadfield, J. D., Hedges, L. V., Held, L., Ho, T.--H., Hoijtink, H., Jones, J. H., Hruschka, D. J., Imai, K., Imbens, G., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Jeon, M., Kirchler, M., Laibson, D., List, J., Little, R., Lupia, A., Machery, E., Maxwell, S. E., McCarthy, M., Moore, D., Morgan, S. L., Munafó, M., Nakagawa, S., Nyhan, B., Parker, T. H., Pericchi, L., Perugini, M., Rouder, J., Rousseau, J., Savalei, V., Schönbrodt, F. D., Sellke, T., Sinclair, B., Tingley, D., Van Zandt, T., Vazire, S., Watts, D. J., Winship, C., Wolpert, R. L., Xie, Y., Young, C., Zinman, J., & Johnson, V. E. (2017). Redefine Statistical Significance. Nature Human Behaviour. http://rdcu.be/wEgG.
- Berker, S. (2009). The normative insignificance of neuroscience. Philosophy & Public Affairs, 37, 293–329. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Boroditsky, L., Fuhrman, O., & McCormick, K. (2011). Do English and Mandarin speakers think about time differently? Cognition, 118, 123–129. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Carter, E. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2013). Is ego depletion too incredible? Evidence for the overestimation of the depletion effect. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 683–684. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Carter, E. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2014). Publication bias and the limited strength model of self-control: Has the evidence for ego depletion been overestimated? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 823. PubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Chen, J. Y. (2007). Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 427–436. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Copp, D. (2007). Morality in a natural world. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. BookGoogle Scholar
- Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, rationality and the human brain. New York: Putnam. Google Scholar
- Daniels, N. (2013). Reflective equilibrium. In W. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Winter 2013 Edition). http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2013/entries/reflective-equilibrium/.
- Darwall, S., Gibbard, A., & Railton, P. (1992). Toward fin de siecle ethics: Some trends. The Philosophical Review, 115–189. Google Scholar
- Doris, J. M. (1998). Persons, situations, and virtue ethics. Noûs, 32, 504–530. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Doris, J. M. (2002). Lack of character: Personality and moral behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press. BookGoogle Scholar
- Doris, J. M. (2015). Talking to our selves: Reflection, ignorance, and agency. Oxford: Oxford University Press. BookGoogle Scholar
- Doris, J. M., & Stich, S. P. (2005). As a matter of fact: Empirical perspectives on ethics. In F. Jackson & M. Smith (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of contemporary philosophy. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar
- Doris, J. M., Machery, E., & Stich, S. (2017). Can psychologists tell us anything about morality? The Philosophers’ Magazine, (77), 24–29. Google Scholar
- Duarte, J. L., Crawford, J. T., Stern, C., Haidt, J., Jussim, L., & Tetlock, P. E. (2015). Political diversity will improve social psychological science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, 1–13. Google Scholar
- Duke, A. A., & Bègue, L. (2015). The drunk utilitarian: Blood alcohol concentration predicts utilitarian responses in moral dilemmas. Cognition, 134, 121–127. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Dunning, D. (1999). A newer look: Motivated social cognition and the schematic representation of social concepts. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 1–11. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Earp, B. D., Everett, J. A. C., Madva, E. N., & Hamlin, J. K. (2014). Out, damned spot: Can the “Macbeth Effect” be replicated? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 36, 91–98. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Fanelli, D. (2010). Positive. Results increase down the hierarchy of the sciences. PloS One, 5, e10068. ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Farah, M. J., & Hook, C. J. (2013). The seductive allure of “seductive allure”. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 88–90. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Fayard, J. V., Bassi, A. K., Bernstein, D. M., & Roberts, B. W. (2009). Is cleanliness next to godliness? Dispelling old wives’ tales: Failure to replicate Zhong and Liljenquist (2006). Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis, 6, 21–30. Google Scholar
- Fernandez-Duque, D., Evans, J., Christian, C., & Hodges, S. D. (2015). Superfluous neuroscience information makes explanations of psychological phenomena more appealing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 926–944. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- FitzPatrick, W. J. (2014). Skepticism about naturalizing normativity. Res Philosophica, 91(4), 559–588. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Fraley, R. C., & Vazire, S. (2014). The N-pact factor: Evaluating the quality of empirical journals with respect to sample size and statistical power. PloS One, 9, e109019. ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Funder, D. C. (2012). The personality puzzle (6th ed.). New York: W.W. Norton. Google Scholar
- Gendler, T. S. (2011). On the epistemic costs of implicit bias. Philosophical Studies, 156(1), 33–63. Google Scholar
- Gilovich, T. (1991). How we know what isn’t so: The fallibility of human reason in everyday life. New York: The Free Press. Google Scholar
- Glymour, C., & Hanson, C. (2016). Reverse inference in neuropsychology. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science 67, 1139–1153. Google Scholar
- Greene, J. D. (2013). Moral tribes: Emotion, reason and the gap between us and them. London: Penguin Press. Google Scholar
- Greene, J. D. (2014). Beyond point-and-shoot morality: Why cognitive (neuro)science matters for ethics. Ethics, 124, 695–726. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Griggs, R. (2015). Coverage of the Phineas Gage story in introductory psychology textbooks: Was gage no longer gage? Teaching of Psychology, 42(3), 195–202. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 495–525. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Harman, G. (1999). Moral philosophy meets social psychology: Virtue ethics and the fundamental attribution error. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 99, 315–331. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Harman, G. (2000). The nonexistence of character traits. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 100, 223–226. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Huang, J. L. (2014). Does cleanliness influence moral judgments? Response effort moderates the effect of cleanliness priming on moral judgments. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1276. ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- January, D., & Kako, E. (2007). Re-evaluating evidence for linguistic relativity: Reply to Boroditsky (2001). Cognition, 104, 417–426. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion—Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulation. Psychological Science, 21, 1686–1693. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Johnson, D. J., Cheung, F., & Donnellan, M. B. (2014). Does cleanliness influence moral judgments? Social Psychology, 45, 209–215. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Johnson, D. J., Wortman, J., Cheung, F., Hein, M., Lucas, R. E., Donnellan, M. B., et al. (2016). The effects of disgust on moral judgments: Testing moderators. Social Psychological and Personality Science , 7(7), 640–647. Google Scholar
- Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480–498. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Lakens, D., Adolfi, F. G., Albers, C. J., Anvari, F., Apps, M. A. J., Argamon, S. E., … Zwaan, R. A. (2017). Justify Your Alpha: A Response to “Redefine Statistical Significance”. Retrieved from psyarxiv.com/9s3y6
- Levy, N. (2011). Resisting ‘weakness of the will’. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 82, 134–155. ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Lurquin, J. H., Michaelson, L. E., Barker, J. E., Gustavson, D. E., von Bastian, C. C., et al. (2016). No evidence of the ego-depletion effect across task characteristics and individual differences: A pre-registered study. PloS One, 11, e0147770. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0147770. ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Machery, E. (2012a). Delineating the moral domain. The Baltic International Yearbook of Cognition, Logic and Communication, 7. doi:10.4148/biyclc.v7i0.1777.
- Machery, E. (2012b). Power and negative results. Philosophy of Science, 79, 808–820. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Machery, E. (2014). In defense of reverse inference. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 65, 251–267. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Machery, E. (in press). Morality: A historical invention. In K. Gray & J. Graham (Eds.), The atlas of moral psychology. New York: The Guilford Press. Google Scholar
- Macmillan, M. (2002). An odd kind of fame: Stories of Phineas Gage. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar
- McShane, B. B., Gal, D., Gelman, A., Robert, C., & Tackett, J. L. (2017). Abandon Statistical Significance. arXiv preprint arXiv:1709.07588. Google Scholar
- Meyer, A., Frederick, S., Burnham, T. C., Guevara Pinto, J. D., Boyer, T. W., Ball, L. J., . & Schuldt, J. P. (2015). Disfluent fonts don’t help people solve math problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(2), e16. Google Scholar
- Nagel, T. (1980). Ethics as an autonomous theoretical subject. In G. S. Stent (Ed.), Morality as a biological phenomenon: The presuppositions of sociobiological research (pp. 198–205). Berkeley; Los Angeles: University of California Press. Google Scholar
- Nguyen, H. H. D., & Ryan, A. M. (2008). Does stereotype threat affect test performance of minorities and women? A meta-analysis of experimental evidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 1314–1334. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Oakes, M. W. (1986). Statistical inference. Epidemiology Resources. Google Scholar
- Peters, D. P., & Ceci, S. J. (1982). Peer-review practices of psychological journals: The fate of published articles, submitted again. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5(02), 187–195. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Can cognitive processes be inferred from neuroimaging data? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 59–63. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Railton, P. (2003). Facts, values, and norms: Essays toward a morality of consequence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. BookGoogle Scholar
- Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Google Scholar
- Robinson, P. H., & Darley, J. M. (1995). Justice, liability, and blame: Community views and the criminal law. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. Google Scholar
- Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. Google Scholar
- Russell, G. (2010). In defence of Hume’s law. In C. Pidgen (Ed.), Hume, is and ought: New essays (pp. 151–161). Hampshire: Palgrave MacMillan. ChapterGoogle Scholar
- Schnall, S., Benton, J., & Harvey, S. (2008). With a clean conscience cleanliness reduces the severity of moral judgments. Psychological Science, 19, 1219–1222. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (1989). Do studies of statistical power have an effect on the power of studies? Psychological Bulletin, 105, 309–316. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Seyedsayamdost, H. (2014). Reproducibility of empirical findings: Experiments in philosophy and beyond. Doctoral dissertation, The London School of Economics and Political Science (LSE). Google Scholar
- Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22, 1359–1366. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Spencer, S. J., Steele, C. M., & Quinn, D. M. (1999). Stereotype threat and women's math performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 4–28. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Stoet, G., & Geary, D. C. (2012). Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematics performance and achievement? Review of General Psychology, 16, 93–102. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Sytsma, J., & Machery, E. (2010). Two conceptions of subjective experience. Philosophical Studies, 151, 299–327. ArticleGoogle Scholar
- Sytsma, J., & Machery, E. (2012). On the relevance of folk intuitions: A commentary on Talbot. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 654–660. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Talbot, B. (2012). The irrelevance of folk intuitions to the “hard problem” of consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 644–650. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Vaillant, G. (2012). The triumphs of experience. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press. BookGoogle Scholar
- Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Manipulations of emotional context shape moral judgment. Psychological Science, 17, 476–477. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Weisberg, D. S., Keil, F. C., Goodstein, J., Rawson, E., & Gray, J. R. (2008). The seductive allure of neuroscience explanations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 470–477. ArticlePubMedPubMed CentralGoogle Scholar
- Williams, B. A. O. (1985). Ethics and the limits of philosophy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Google Scholar
- Williams, W. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2015). National hiring experiments reveal 2: 1 faculty preference for women on STEM tenure track. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(17), 5360–5365. Google Scholar
- Zhong, C. B., & Liljenquist, K. (2006). Washing away your sins: Threatened morality and physical cleansing. Science, 313, 1451–1452. ArticlePubMedGoogle Scholar
- Zhong, C. B., Strejcek, B., & Sivanathan, N. (2010). A clean self can render harsh moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 859–862. ArticleGoogle Scholar
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
- Center for Philosophy of Science, University of Pittsburgh, 817 CL, Pittsburgh, PA, 15260, USA Edouard Machery
- Philosophy-Neuroscience-Psychology Program & Philosophy Department, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, USA John M. Doris
- Edouard Machery